雅思考试小白入门指南
2019-06-25
更新时间:2022-10-11 21:00:06作者:佚名
如果大家想要在托福考试中取得一个好成绩,那么就一定要重视阅读,怎样才能考出好成绩呢?接下来就和出国留学网看看2018年11月24日托福阅读考试机经。
Vocalization in Frogs
The Túngara frog is a small terrestrial vertebrate that is found in Central America. Túngara frogs breed in small pools, and breeding groups range from a single male to choruses of several hundred males. The advertisement call of a male Túngara frog is a strange noise, a whine that starts at a frequency of 900 hertz and sweeps downward to 400 hertz in about 400 milliseconds. The whine may be produced by itself, or it may be followed by one or several chucks or clucking sounds. When a male Túngara frog is calling alone in a pond, it usually gives only the whine portion of the call, but as additional males join a chorus, more and more of the frogs produce calls that include chucks. Scientists noted that male Túngara frogs calling in a breeding pond added chucks to their calls when they heard the recorded calls of other males played back. That observation suggested that it was the presence of other calling males that incited frogs to make their calls more complex by adding chucks to the end of the whine.
What advantage would a male frog in a chorus gain from using a whine-chuck call instead of a whine?
Perhaps the complex call is more attractive to female frogs than the simple call. Michael Ryan and Stanley Rand tested that hypothesis by placing female Túngara frogs in a test arena with a speaker at each side. One speaker broadcast a pre-recorded whine call, and the second speaker broadcast a whine-chuck. When female frogs were released individually in the center of the arena, fourteen of the fifteen frogs tested moved toward the speaker broadcasting the whine-chuck call.
If female frogs are attracted to whine-chuck calls in preference to whine calls, why do male frogs give whine-chuck calls only when other males are present? Why not always give the most attractive call possible? One possibility is that whine-chuck calls require more energy than whines, and males save energy by only using whine-chucks when competition with other males makes the energy expenditure necessary. However, measurements of the energy expenditure of calling male Túngara frogs showed that energy cost was not related to the number of chucks. Another possibility is that male frogs giving whine- chuck calls are more vulnerable to predators than frogs giving only whine calls. Túngara frogs in breeding choruses are preyed upon by a species of frog-eating bats, Trachops cirrhosus, and it was demonstrated that the bats locate the frogs by homing on their vocalizations.
In a series of playback experiments, Michael Ryan and Merlin Tuttle placed pairs of speakers in the forest and broadcast vocalizations of Túngara frogs. One speaker played a recording of a whine and the other a recording of a whine-chuck. The bats responded as if the speakers were frogs: they flew toward the speakers and even landed on them. In five experiments at different sites, the bats approached speakers broadcasting whine-chuck calls twice as frequently as those playing simple whines (168 approaches versus 81). Thus, female frogs are not alone in finding whine-chuck calls more attractive than simple whines. An important predator of frogs also responds more strongly to the complex calls.
Ryan and his colleagues measured the rates of predation in Túngara frog choruses of different sizes. Large choruses of frogs did not attract more bats than small choruses, and consequently the risk of predation for
an individual frog was less in a large chorus than in a small one. Predation was an astonishing 19 percent of the frogs per night in the smallest chorus and a substantial 1.5 percent per night even in the largest chorus. When a male frog shifts from a simple whine to a whine-chuck call, it increases its chances of attracting a female, but it simultaneously increases its risk of attracting a predator. In small choruses, the competition from other males for females is relatively small, and the risk of predation is relatively large.
Under these conditions it is apparently advantageous for a male Túngara frog to give simple whines. However, as chorus size increases, competition with other males also increases while the risk of predation falls. In that situation, the advantage of giving a complex call apparently outweighs the risks.
用户评论
终于找到真题了!现在正在苦攻TPO和模拟试题,这部分内容对冲刺分数特别有用。希望这回可以考出一个理想的成绩,毕竟要准备申请学校的时间越来越紧迫。
有20位网友表示赞同!
看到这个托福阅读机经真是太幸福了!考试前两天我总是很忐忑不安,这次真题经验能让我更自信,感觉把握住了考试节奏也更加清晰。不过希望这篇文章能够详细介绍一下各个方面的考点变化,比如题目类型、文章内容难度等等。
有16位网友表示赞同!
这个机经真的太好了!感谢分享!最近一直在刷TPO,有些地方还是觉得不够熟悉,这篇机经可以帮助我们更好地理解考试规律和重点突破方向。希望以后还能继续更新其他时间段的机经,毕竟托福考试频率较高需要及时掌握最新的考点变化。
有12位网友表示赞同!
这个机经的内容是不是有点水?我感觉写的太简了,没有分析具体的题型或是文章风格特点。如果能详细讲解一下每篇阅读内容的主旨和一些难点所在,那肯定更有帮助。
有15位网友表示赞同!
这个托福阅读考试机经让我觉得有点鸡肋,因为很多地方都比较模棱两可,没有给出明确的答案或分析,反而容易给我带来更多困惑。还是建议大家多关注官方提供的练习资料和真题库,避免盲目地参考此类总结性内容。
有13位网友表示赞同!
这份机经对我来说真是一个福音!现在我在备考阶段,想要通过托福阅读考试,真的很需要这种实践性的学习资料。希望这份机经能够帮助我理解考试重点,掌握答题技巧,最终拿到理想的成绩。
有15位网友表示赞同!
2018年的机经还是很有参考价值的! 很多文章内容和考试内容相仿,虽然时间有些久远了,但依然可以积累一些阅读素材和解题思路。
有20位网友表示赞同!
这个机经太少了!我只看到了两篇阅读内容,希望能多添加一些真题数据,这样才能更有针对性地帮助备考人员学习。
有9位网友表示赞同!
这份托福阅读考试机经分享的不错,让我找到了很多宝贵的经验和方法。特别是对某些特定的阅读技巧的讲解,例如快速定位关键词、判断文章主旨等,都让我受益匪浅!
有20位网友表示赞同!
希望这份机经能更新到最新的信息,毕竟考试内容还是会随着时间变化而调整。
有12位网友表示赞同!
这篇文章总结得比较详细,每个步骤都清晰易懂,对于没有接触过托福阅读的同学来说很有帮助。 还有就是希望能多提供一些练习题和答案解析,这样可以更直观地了解到学习方法的有效性。
有11位网友表示赞同!
这个机经能让我更好地了解托福阅读的特点和规律,特别是关于文章结构和写作风格的分析非常有用!希望以后还能更新更多不同类型的机经内容,让大家能够在备考的过程中更加明确目标和方向。
有9位网友表示赞同!
我觉得这种总结性的机经文章本身并不可信,考试真题毕竟会有很多变化,还是建议你多刷官方练习资料或者参加一些培训课程学习更系统的知识点。
有10位网友表示赞同!
机经很实用了!我之前一直觉得托福阅读比较难,看了这篇文章我觉得我的学习方向是正确的。希望以后还有更多更新内容,这样我能更有信心去考试了!
有8位网友表示赞同!
对于托福写作来说这个机经分析还是比较浅层的,并没有深入探讨不同的考题类型和评分标准。 如果能结合一些优秀例文进行分析,对备考者更加有帮助!
有7位网友表示赞同!
我感觉这种回顾性的文章有点没用了,考试内容总是会变化的!建议大家多关注最新的考试趋势和官方发布的信息,才能真正掌握考试规律。
有19位网友表示赞同!